©

Ulrike Domahs from unibz’s Faculty of Education is poised to open a new lab for Cognitive Educational Science that will allow EEG researchers to delve further into the mysterious enigma that is the human brain.

People think we can read their minds with it, but we can’t!” Ulrike Domahs is talking about the EEG machine—those not-so-stylish skullcaps that measure the brain’s electrical activity through electrodes placed on the scalp. But unlike a creepy scene from a sci-fi movie, Domahs’ use of the machine is giving us elegant (and totally un-scary) insight into how the human brain processes language. Domahs is a German-language professor from the unibz Faculty of Education and is the Director of the new Cognitive Educational Science (CES) Laboratory in Brixen/Bressanone. The lab is open to all researchers interested in studying the cognitive processes involved in the 20 million billion electrical commands that our brain’s neurons are sending out every second.

Personally, I prefer them on my feet

An electroencephalograph (or EEG) represents this neural chatter as waves. The technology is not new by any means. Back in 1929, a “brooding and introverted” German scientist, Hans Berger, watched as his rudimentary EEG scratched out the first inky-black brain wave of a 17-year-old college student by the name of Zedel. Within a few decades the EEG had become the staple tool of cognitive science. Scientists eventually identified six speeds of brainwaves—infra-low, delta, theta, alpha, beta and gamma—an orchestra of low to high bandwidths whose interactions are fundamental to understanding neurological disorders, sleep patterns and language. Ulrike Domahs’ research is based on an application of EEG technology called Event-Related Potentials (ERP). This approach measures the time it takes for the brain to react to a stimulus or event and the extent of that reaction. For example, if a friend tells you: “My wife spread butter and socks on her toast this morning”, your brain will register a negative shift on the EEG recording at about 400 milliseconds after hearing “socks” (the so-called N-400 effect). By comparing the N-400s of many individuals, Domahs can see patterns in how the brain processes language under different conditions.

Our Brainy Tongues

“We used to think about language processing from the monolingual adult’s perspective. Therefore, it was conceived of being quite uniform across people”, says Domahs of her work. “But we now know that it is affected by the amount of experience you have with a language. Therefore, it is important to know what’s going on in the brains of those learning a language, as well as those who are learning more than one language.” To get an overview of this area of language processing, Domahs has performed ERP studies with many different tongues— Turkish, Arabic, Italian, English, German, to name a few—as well as many different age groups. In particular, she has worked with children to observe the development of their mental lexicons, the brain’s piggybank of information about a word’s meaning, pronunciation, and other qualities. She also looked at how children use language rhythm to process words with different grammatical endings. (An example is the singular and plural forms of the German for dogs, cats and birds: Hund vs Hund(e), Katze vs Katze(n) or Vogel vs V(ö) gel.) In addition, she has worked with children with language impairment as well as with adults with a history of developmental language impairment to find out how language processing is affected by language disorders. Domahs will soon be comparing how Italian-German bilinguals and German monolinguals process language rhythm to see if the processing of rhythm changes when Italian and German speech rhythms coexist. Research such as this is important in South Tyrol, where questions of second-language acquisition are ever-present. We still don’t know enough about the difference between mono- and bilingual language processing, but whatever language or languages there are in our noggins, Domahs’ research may shape the way we teach them them in the future.

Related Articles

Article
Mummy research: Ancient dental calculus - new insights into the evolution of oral microbiota

A research team from Eurac Research and the University of Trento has examined ancient calculus samples from skeletal remains from South Tyrol and Trentino and discovered previously unknown species of microorganisms.

Article
Grüne Lösungen für klimafreundlichere, lebenswertere Städte

Bozen und Meran sind Pilotgebiete in einem von Eurac Research koordinierten EU-Projekt, das neue Wege erforscht, mit Hilfe der Natur urbanen Herausforderungen zu begegnen.

Article
Ricerca identifica i tre fattori principali che controllano il comportamento e le risposte degli ecosistemi terrestri

Un gruppo di ricercatori internazionali, guidato dal Max Planck Institute for Biogeochemistry (Jena, Germania) e comprendente Libera Università di Bolzano e Ripartizione Foreste della Provincia di Bolzano oltre a ARPA Valle d’Aosta, Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche, Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca e Università degli Studi della Tuscia di Viterbo, ha identificato tre funzioni chiave che governano il comportamento degli ecosistemi e che sono legate alla loro efficienza nell’utilizzo di carbonio e acqua. L’osservazione di queste tre funzioni chiave permetterà di monitorare e studiare il comportamento degli ecosistemi terrestri e di capire la loro sensibilità ai cambiamenti climatici e ambientali in atto, contribuendo all’ottimizzazione della loro gestione.

Article
Mobilità elettrica: la svolta è cominciata

Un nuovo dossier di Eurac Research fa il punto di finanze, tecnologie e politiche sulla base delle ricerche più attuali.