Demis Basso from the unibz Faculty of Education is looking at the development of a number of cognitive processes in children to help teachers to better evaluate their pupils. Could a child’s slower cognitive development result in poor social and scholarly performance?

Ten-year-old Suzie puts her head on her desk and peers through her fingers. She’s hoping her teacher won’t make her line up behind her classmates putting their math assignments on his desk. But clever Mr. Guenther has already seen her in the crowd. “I forgot to do it,” says Suzie sheepishly, knowing his question before he asks it. The man etches a note in his journal: it’s the third time this month. He knows he has to write to her parents about her motivation. What Guenther doesn’t know is that Suzie’s cognitive functions are developing at a different rate than her peers: it’s not that she’s unmotivated, she simply can’t remember. If he knew, would he evaluate her differently? This is what Demis Basso, a cognitive scientist at the unibz Faculty of Education, wonders. Basso wants to help schoolteachers better understand the social implications of a little-known cognitive function called ‘prospective memory.’

A Memory for the Future

“When we talk about memory,” says Basso, “we tend to think about retrieving facts or events from the past. This is called ‘retrospective’ memory. It is a cognitive function that stores information in either long-term or short-term systems. For example, ‘I remember that my mop broke yesterday.’ Prospective memory, on the other hand, is a cognitive function necessary to store plans and recall them at a specific time or when specific cues are present. ‘On the way home, when I pass through town, I must remember to stop to get a new mop.’” In other words, prospective memory is about remembering to remember. Aristotle called it a “memory for the future.” The important social impact of prospective memory piqued Basso’s interest: if a person’s retrospective memory fails, we generally perceive the person’s memory to be at fault; but if their prospective memory fails, the person is seen as unmotivated or ‘flaky.’ “The development of prospective memory is based on the functionality of the frontal lobe, which only develops completely by the age of 12 to 13,” explains Basso. “But every child develops at his or her own pace. If a child doesn’t do their homework, is it because they were unmotivated or is it simply because they forgot because their memory has not developed sufficiently?”

“Hey! Teacher!”

With the collaboration of Milvia Cottini, Basso examined prospective memory and the perception of a child’s social skills via evaluations by the child and his or her teacher.The assessment of 74 school-aged children supported the hypothesis that the children’s marks were a significant predictor of social evaluations provided by the teacher (i.e., tendency towards aggression); the lower the marks, the lower the social skills. And with prospective memory tasks and teacher evaluations, he found something every child and parent should take note of: the lower the prospective memory, the lower the marks. “The study suggests that teachers—as well as other non-specialists—are usually not able to distinguish between cognitive maturation and social skills, meaning that it is likely that prospective memory skills are one of the factors that affect a child’s grades and social behaviour.” It’s not that teachers have to change the way they teach, underlines Basso, but rather they should pay attention to the ability of the child to complete that task and propose different strategies. “If you know a child is weak in the prospective memory process, you must take more care about the way you give the tasks. Otherwise, we might incorrectly assess the child as unmotivated, which creates its own kind of social stigma. It is really important to understand more about cognitive processes and how they can be used to improve our on-the-job performance. ”

Related Articles

Article
Mummy research: Ancient dental calculus - new insights into the evolution of oral microbiota

A research team from Eurac Research and the University of Trento has examined ancient calculus samples from skeletal remains from South Tyrol and Trentino and discovered previously unknown species of microorganisms.

Article
Grüne Lösungen für klimafreundlichere, lebenswertere Städte

Bozen und Meran sind Pilotgebiete in einem von Eurac Research koordinierten EU-Projekt, das neue Wege erforscht, mit Hilfe der Natur urbanen Herausforderungen zu begegnen.

Article
Ricerca identifica i tre fattori principali che controllano il comportamento e le risposte degli ecosistemi terrestri

Un gruppo di ricercatori internazionali, guidato dal Max Planck Institute for Biogeochemistry (Jena, Germania) e comprendente Libera Università di Bolzano e Ripartizione Foreste della Provincia di Bolzano oltre a ARPA Valle d’Aosta, Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche, Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca e Università degli Studi della Tuscia di Viterbo, ha identificato tre funzioni chiave che governano il comportamento degli ecosistemi e che sono legate alla loro efficienza nell’utilizzo di carbonio e acqua. L’osservazione di queste tre funzioni chiave permetterà di monitorare e studiare il comportamento degli ecosistemi terrestri e di capire la loro sensibilità ai cambiamenti climatici e ambientali in atto, contribuendo all’ottimizzazione della loro gestione.

Article
Mobilità elettrica: la svolta è cominciata

Un nuovo dossier di Eurac Research fa il punto di finanze, tecnologie e politiche sulla base delle ricerche più attuali.